Two Mozart Fingerprints
Related to the Becking Conducting Curve
by Nigel Nettheim ©2003
University of Western Sydney
[Manuscript completed 26 July 2000,
this web version posted 25 January 2003,
revised 31 January 2003.]
ABSTRACT
Composer-specific conducting curves were proposed by Becking in 1928
on a somewhat intuitive basis.
This paper seeks properties of the scores
that can be matched more explicitly to Becking's curve for Mozart.
Systematic score study requires first an orderly treatment of musical metres,
especially in terms of the number (B/C) of notated bars per conducting curve.
Short compositional patterns are next proposed as "fingerprints"
characterising Mozart's musical personality;
these are documented by many score excerpts which are aligned vertically for
metrical comparison.
An attempt is made to match the notes of the excerpts approximately
to corresponding locations on the conducting curve.
Tempo indications seen in the excerpts are compared with the value of B/C,
suggesting that Mozart generally intended his indications
to be applied to notated bars rather than to conducting curves.
The above results are compared with those obtained in earlier work for Schubert.
The comparison between Mozart and Haydn is briefly treated.
On the whole, the evidence found in the scores
seems to support both Becking's approach and the notion of fingerprint.
CONTENTS
1. Introduction
Par 1
This paper constitutes a step along the way towards a larger objective:
to differentiate selected composers' musical personalities
on the basis of a study of their scores.
The selected composers are ones usually considered to be "great"
(which might be defined as those who have been able
to embody in their music a distinctive personality
that is considered to have general value).
Other composers, who may have adopted a more formulaic or prosaic
approach to composition, perhaps deriving their work from the examples
of the front-runners,
will not be dealt with,
for to distinguish
their work from that of the leaders
is a special task that would require a close examination
in each of the innumerable cases,
and by definition results obtained for those composers would have less value.
We are thus spared the impossible task of dealing with all of music;
but the amount to be dealt with is nevertheless very large.
Par 2
To assist in processing the large amount of data,
the notion of a musical "fingerprint" will be adopted.
A fingerprint of a given composer is defined here as a
compositional pattern occurring in many of the composer's
scores and in few scores of other composers;
it will be carefully specified in each case
and comprises a few notes typically covering about one bar of music.
It may happen that several such patterns are found for a given composer
and act jointly to distinguish his music from that of other
composers.
1
Further, a fingerprint might not occur often in any one movement,
for a single occurrence can place its stamp on a whole movement.
Par 3
It is desired not only to find and document composers' fingerprints,
which may be of interest for their own sake,
but also to relate these to
Becking's (1928)
notion of composer-specific conducting shapes.
That notion, which will be explained more fully in the next section,
has allowed the search for fingerprints to
proceed in a directed rather than a haphazard manner.
The conducting shape is considered as not fully revealed in any one score
examined in isolation, but as latent in the composer's collected scores.
Becking dealt on a more or less intuitive basis
with several well-chosen individual excerpts
but not with a composer's output as a whole,
so he was not in a position to point to specific features of the scores
that imply the shape of a curve.
Evidence implying the composer's conducting curve
is deeply hidden in his scores, and involves innumerable features of them;
the present attempt to make some of that evidence explicit
can hope to produce only relatively crude results,
yet the attempt seems worth making.
It should be noted that it is not sufficient for the present purpose
to find general features typical of the composer's output—for instance,
the feature of Mozart's music that its textures are especially spare—for
while such features may imply something of a curve's quality
they would not imply its shape.
Par 4
A fingerprint was recently proposed for Schubert
and documented in 150 excerpts from his works
(Nettheim, 1998).
That fingerprint was related to Becking's conducting curve for Schubert
by matching various features of the excerpts to corresponding features of
the curve.
A theoretical treatment of metre dealt with
the number of bars per curve (B/C), that is,
the number of 'nominal' bars
(as printed) per 'real' bar (corresponding to one complete conducting curve),
a number which needs to be determined if the music is to be understood in
conducting terms; implications for the understanding of Schubert's tempo
indications were also explored.
Par 5
The present paper continues that line of research
by proposing two Mozart fingerprints.
Much of the theoretical background was already detailed in the Schubert paper
(Nettheim, ibid),
so it will be covered a little less fully here.
As two composers, Schubert and Mozart, have now been studied in this way,
systematic comparison between them is possible and will be pursued.
On the other hand, the interesting question of the separation of the Mozart
conducting beat from the Haydn one via fingerprints is not a main concern
at this stage and will be looked at only briefly.
Implications for tempo in performance will follow from the study
of the relationship between tempo indications and the number
B/C,
but implications for the detailed nuances of performance
within a given tempo are held over for future
work.
2
It should finally be mentioned that the present study is a musicological one;
in particular, the point of view of cognitive psychology is not
taken up here.
3
2. The Becking Curve
Par 6
The central aspect from which music is to be studied here is
the conducting shape which it may induce.
According to this approach, associated with a piece or movement of music
and running throughout it (with occasional possible exceptions)
is a more or less exactly repeated cycle of pressure
somewhat analogous to the human pulse.
Some people may feel such a cycle internally when in the presence of the music
(whether by listening, performing, or silent
score-reading).
4
The cycle runs not only through notes but also through rests.
This notion was thoroughly treated by
Becking (1928),
for which English-language synopses were provided by
Shove & Repp (1995)
and
Nettheim (1996).
To render the internal feeling externally visible,
Becking used a small baton, letting the music itself induce the motion,
a process which I have suggested calling 'armchair' conducting.
It is important to note that the causal direction is thus reversed
from the usual one:
whereas in an actual performance the conductor influences the rendition,
here the musical composition influences the conducting.
Becking found that the curves observed differ markedly between
composers, nationalities and historical times.
Par 7
Becking presented two-dimensional diagrams
which became known as 'Becking curves',
indicating the way his small baton moved
in response to the music of each of a number of composers.
His curve for Mozart is shown in Figure 1,
which gives the view as seen by a right-handed subject.
The varying thickness of the curve represents the varying strength
applied in the beating
(taking into account both agonist and antagonist muscles),
and Becking added the verbal description
"selbstverständlich abwärts, sorgfältig getönt"
(naturally down, carefully shaded),
but the varying velocity is not shown graphically.
Several distinguishable phases have been indicated along the curve
by the present writer for later use:
the (D)ownbeat, the move (A)round the bottom,
the following (N)otch and finally the (U)pbeat.
The motion of the baton starts high on the left side
just before the top of the Figure and moves up to the top
in a brief preparation for the downbeat which follows.
The downbeat falls on the right side freely at first
and is then more finely controlled for the careful swing
around the lowest area.
Next, after some height has been gained,
a small rounded notch is traced out forming a secondary low region,
not as low as before;
this phase is quite refined and may be found to a certain extent delectable.
This notching movement creates enough momentum to allow an easy continuation
for the upbeat which completes the cycle.
The shape is intended to be executed and felt,
rather than merely viewed on paper.
A more detailed description, especially of the downbeat,
is given by
Becking (ibid, pp. 23-26)
and is outlined in translation in
Nettheim (1996, pp. 113-115);
an attempt at an animation of the curve is made in
Nettheim (2003).
Fig. 1. The Becking curve for Mozart (Becking, 1928, end-plate).
Curve regions have been indicated by the present writer.
Par 8
It is natural to ask whether Becking's prescription
for Mozart's conducting curve has an underlying authenticity,
or whether it instead represents just Becking's personal response to the music.
The present study adopts Becking's prescription as an hypothesis
in order to pursue its implications and to test it against the scores.
That prescription happens to ring true for the present writer,
and it will be confirmed to a considerable extent by the work to follow.
On the other hand, the successful proposal of an alternative curve prescription
is theoretically conceivable;
its results could be pursued by the same method as that which will be used here.
Par 9
The present approach clearly requires a decision on where in the score
each conducting curve begins and ends,
and those places are not always obvious.
It is therefore necessary to determine, as far as possible,
the number of printed bars per curve (B/C).
That quantity, which I will call the curve's 'scope',
can in practice take the values 1, ½ or 2
(or perhaps very rarely another value).
In this connection I define a 'real' bar as covering one curve,
by contrast with a 'nominal' bar as printed;
the scope, B/C, is then the number of 'nominal' bars per 'real' bar.
Various possible methods for determining the scope were mentioned
in
Nettheim (1998, section 1);
the evidence of carefully defined fingerprints was the main one used there,
as will be the case here too.
5
To attempt such determinations,
we first need to deal with the question of the metrical representation
of the music in the score.
3. Metrical representation of the music in the score
Par 10
Any metre appearing in a score can be regarded as resulting
from three choices:
(i) a 'real' metre,
(ii) the number (1, ½ or 2) of notated or 'nominal' bars per curve, and
(iii) the note-value chosen as metrical unit.
When (i), a 'real' metre, is given, then (ii) and (iii) determine the
'nominal' metre. These choices will now be explored systematically.
Par 11
Two given metres may be essentially different,
as for instance 3/4 vs 4/4, or 3/4 vs 6/8.
They may instead be non-essentially different,
differing just in manner of notation,
as for instance 3/4
vs 3/8, or one bar of 6/8 vs two bars of 3/8.
This is not to say that there is then
no difference between the metres;
thus 3/8 might be considered to imply a lighter style of performance than 3/4,
and perhaps a quicker
tempo.
6
The terms 'essentially different' and 'non-essentially different' apply, then,
to pairs of metres,
whereas the terms 'real' and 'nominal' apply to a single metre.
Par 12
The notion adopted here is that a given (continuous) underlying curve
can be manifested in different (discrete) metres.
That was to be expected in case the difference between such metres is
non-essential.
It is, however, true also in the case of essentially different metres:
although the corresponding conducting shapes might not then be identical,
they will be similar.
7
This follows directly from Becking's notion of the curve,
for a composer is by no means restricted to just one 'real' metre.
It is natural, though, that each composer may show some preference for the
'real' metre or metres in which he can best represent his conducting curve.
He may also take his own attitude to the conventions of 'nominal' metre,
as well as of the related tempo indications.
Thus we must discard text-book approaches to metre,
which generally do not distinguish between composers,
and instead build up our understanding empirically for each composer
separately.
8
Mozart's 'real' metres
Par 13
It turns out that for Mozart, as was found also for Schubert,
most of the ground can be covered with just three categories of 'real' metre:
simple duple/quadruple, simple triple, and compound duple.
The distinction between simple duple and simple quadruple metre
in respect of the conducting curve and its representation in the scores,
though certainly perceptible, is relatively slight,
and will accordingly only be commented upon in passing.
Excerpts in different 'real' metres
are related to the Mozart curve in Figure 2.
Fig. 2. Excerpts, in the three main essentially different metres,
related to Becking's Mozart curve:
Par 14
Staff A shows an excerpt chosen to represent Mozart's 4/4 conducting shape:
the beginning of the Piano Sonata K545-1
(the left hand accompaniment is not shown here,
but see later
Figure 3).
The tempo can provisionally be taken to lie within a range of about 1.5 to
2.0 seconds per curve.
When the music is heard or thought an internal feeling is induced,
whose corresponding conducting shape may be followed externally.
The matching of the curve to the score between Figures 1 and 2
is indicated by means of circled initial letters in each Figure.
The letters in the score are intended to correspond only approximately
to the start of the indicated phases, not to indicate precise locations
(precision has been attempted in
Nettheim (2003)
but the finer detail is not needed for the present purpose).
Indeed,
Becking (ibid, p.24) wrote that the downbeat
in 3/4 metre lasted for "something less than" ("etwas weniger als")
two quarter-notes, again only an approximate indication;
the (A) phase is shown in the score commencing a little after
the 2nd quarter-note of the bar,
subsequent to the carefully executed lower portion of the curve's downbeat.
The approximate matching was feasible in this case only because two conditions
were satisfied:
(i) the number of printed bars per curve was assumed known (B/C = 1) and
(ii) the excerpt was both typical of the composer and comparatively simple;
for many excerpts the matching would be less transparent.
Par 15
Staff B shows an attempt by the present writer to devise music
similar to that in Staff A and
yielding as nearly as possible the same conducting curve but now in 3/4,
a metre which is essentially different from the 4/4 of staff A.
To make such an attempt, the first requirement is a change of mental attitude
to instal the new metre with full conviction.
Suitable locations within the bar are then sought for the phases
which were distinguished above.
The order of the phases is of course maintained in the new metre,
but the locations must in some cases be shifted a little,
for the metrical divisions differ essentially.
Comparison is facilitated by the correct alignment of all staves of the Figure
(that is, the spacings are correctly matched for printing,
though not exactly proportional to the durations).
The new rhythms have been determined not by the present writer's intuition
but from a study of Mozart's practice as revealed in his scores,
to be documented later.
It was clear that two short notes should lead either to the 2nd beat
(as 32nd-notes) or to the 3rd beat (as 16th-notes):
the latter was chosen because Mozart more often made that choice,
though he sometimes used the former
(see e.g.
Becking, ibid, p.23,
bar 4 of Example 3a from K202-1).
It is important to note that the (N) and (U) phases,
each of which had in duple metre occurred at its own part of the bar,
now in triple metre occur together in compressed fashion
on the last beat of the bar;
this phenomenon was mentioned specifically by
Becking (ibid, p.39)
(who however did not discuss specifically the conducting of 'real' 6/8 metre).
Par 16
Staff C shows an implementation, once more by the present writer,
in 6/8 metre.
The durations are again taken from Mozart's typical practice,
although other choices might also be feasible.
The rhythm at the beginning of bar 2, for instance,
could conceivably have comprised a dotted quarter-note and two 32nd-notes,
but that would not have corresponded to his practice.
It may be noted that the pair of 16th-notes in 6/8 metre necessarily occupies
a larger proportion of the bar than in 4/4 (one sixth vs one eighth).
The 6/8 implementation may be felt as not quite so natural for the Mozart
Becking curve as was the 4/4 implementation—a certain tautness
has been lost—and indeed 'real' 6/8 metre is apparently used relatively
less by Mozart, as will be documented
later.
9
Par 17
Simple duple metre (2/4) differs in some respects
from simple quadruple metre (4/4),
but not sufficiently to warrant considering it 'essentially' different
for the present purpose.
Manifestations of the two metres in the scores seem to be fully comparable,
as will later be seen, the distinction arising rather in performance:
in 4/4, all four conducting phases are executed distinctly,
whereas in 2/4 (and 2/2) the first two phases are more nearly fused,
as are also the last two phases.
This distinction was described diagrammatically by
Becking (ibid, p.40).
Par 18
In considering the possibility of other 'real' metres,
we note that 'nominal' 3/4 metre could be used with triplet 8th-notes,
as in the Piano Sonata K279-2;
then the 'real' metre could be understood either as 9/8 or as 3/4,
a question not pursued here.
Mozart may exceptionally have used still other 'real' metres,
but we have now covered his main ones.
Mozart's 'nominal' metres
Par 19
Three musical genres were selected for special study in the present paper:
piano sonatas (solo), string quartets, and lieder;
the scores were consulted in the
Bärenreiter
edition.
10
The 'nominal' metres which Mozart used in these genres are shown in Table 1
(other 'nominal' metres than the six appearing there occur very rarely
in Mozart's remaining genres).
In this study each metre/tempo section is regarded as a separate 'segment' of
music, so the total number of segments exceeds the total number of movements;
further, all separately published versions of a movement
are counted separately.
Those factors somewhat bias the Table as an indication of Mozart's practice,
but it is suited to the present different purpose.
Unnotated changes of effective metre such as hemiola may occur,
but these are not tabulated here.
Without yet knowing the 'real' metres applying in each case,
the significance of the Table cannot properly be judged:
it constitutes just a starting point for the investigation.
Table 1. 'Nominal' metres: Number of Tempo/metre Segments
in the Selected Genres of Mozart's Works:
Metre: |
3/4 |
4/4 |
2/4 |
2/2 |
6/8 |
3/8 |
Total |
Segments:
| 62 |
46 |
42 |
37 |
27 |
8 |
222 |
%: |
28 |
20 |
19 |
17 |
12 |
4 |
100 |
Par 20
We now take into account the number of bars per curve (B/C).
A complete tabulation is not possible,
because in some cases that number has not been determined with certainty.
The cases found so far in the genres studied are shown in Table 2;
these cases will be documented shortly in score excerpts in the Figures
indicated in the present Table (
Figures 4a(i) etc.).
No case of B/C = ¼ has been confirmed,
but the Sonata K332-2 might be considered in that respect.
Table 2. 'Nominal' metres having the Indicated 'Real' metre
and Number of Bars per Curve (B/C)
in the Selected Genres of Mozart's Works
(Figure numbers for the Examples given later are also shown):
|
|
'Real' Metre |
|
|
Simple |
Compound |
|
|
Duple / Quadruple |
Triple |
Duple / Quadruple |
Triple |
B/C |
1 |
2/4, 2/2 5a(ii), 5a(iii) 8a(ii), 8a(iii) |
4/4 5a(i) 8a(i) |
3/4, 3/8 5d, . . . 8d, . . . |
6/8 5e 8f |
. . . |
. . . |
½ |
4/4, 2/2 5b 8b |
— |
6/8 . . . 8e |
. . . . . . |
— |
. . . |
2 |
— |
2/4 5c 8c |
— |
3/4, 3/8 . . . 8f |
. . . |
— |
. . . : does not occur in the selected Mozart genres
— : cannot exist
Par 21
A problem arising from Table 2 is that it is quite clear
that Mozart sometimes used 'nominal' 2/2 metre with B/C = ½
(this usage was not found in Schubert
by
Nettheim, 1998).
Each half of the 'nominal' bars can then not represent 'real' 1/2 metre
but must provide at least two divisions of the curve,
that is, two 'nominal' 4th-notes.
The question then arises:
in the cases where B/C = ½,
what difference is intended between 'nominal' 2/2 and 'nominal' 4/4?
It seems from some comparisons of excerpts to be shown later,
containing virtually the same rhythms and very similar music,
that any difference intended is slight.
4. Fingerprints and Prototypes
Par 22
Mozart's extant scores constitute virtually our entire documentary
evidence of his music.
Therefore, if his musical personality is to be revealed anywhere
it must be via his scores, no matter how deeply it may be hidden in them.
As we are here adopting the Becking curve as a governing
notion,
we will search in the scores for evidence of the curve's two key features:
its scope (B/C) and its shape, both defined earlier.
That evidence cannot sufficiently be seen in any one musical score
or excerpt considered by itself, as Becking pointed out
(ibid, p.9).
The only remaining possibility is that the evidence resides in the scores
as a whole œuvre.
11
The rationale to be adopted for the systematic study of the scores
of a given composer is that a number of fingerprint patterns are present
in various of the composer's scores and that,
while no one pattern fully determines or implies the curve's scope and shape,
they do so jointly,
each one making a partial contribution to the determination.
Each such contribution can be expected to focus on just one or a few properties
of the curve.
Moreover, a fingerprint may appear in a work or movement perhaps just once
or a few times, possibly early in it,
suggesting that the whole music is to be understood according to
the curve shape thus indicated more or less briefly but strongly;
the remainder of the music,
although it might further reinforce that indication of shape,
need not do so—instead, it need only be consistent with it.
Par 23
The tasks, then, are
(i) to propose a suitable set of carefully defined fingerprints
for the given composer, and
(ii) to document them as extensively as possible in the scores.
The first of these tasks requires a considerable familiarity with the scores
together with musical understanding
(involving, perhaps, broad and rapid subconscious processing, cf.
Ehrenzweig, 1967).
12
The second task is more purely mechanical,
and the results will therefore be objectively verifiable,
to the extent possible for any results in an artistic discipline.
In particular, a fingerprint falsely proposed will not be capable of
convincing documentation in the scores of the given composer.
Further, the common appearance of a fingerprint of one composer in the scores
of another composer does not necessarily deny the value of that fingerprint
for either composer,
because of the possibility of the joint action of
sets
of fingerprints as mentioned above (
Par 22).
Par 24
A fingerprint is here understood to comprise just a few notes
typically covering about one 'real' bar.
It is not sufficient for the present purpose to find general features
typical of the composer's output—for instance,
the feature of Mozart's music that its textures are especially spare.
Again, prototypical
phrases, as treated by
Gjerdingen (1991),
are much longer portions of music than the single-bar units
that will be considered here,
and they accordingly have very different
properties.
13
Par 25
Prototypes will be used to represent each proposed fingerprint concisely.
They are designed as convenient reference points for the more extensive
documentation to be attempted later.
A given prototype is shown as a set of excerpts,
one excerpt for each 'real' metre.
To facilitate identification, at least one of the prototype excerpts is,
when possible, chosen to be a rather well-known one.
5. The Mozart Fingerprint M1 ('Middle Twiddle')
Prototypes for M1
Par 26
In looking for a first figure suitable as a Mozart fingerprint
I concentrated attention on the characteristic (N)otch phase of the curve,
therefore paying particular attention to the corresponding places in the scores
(certainly there is no comparable phase in the Schubert curve,
shown later in
Figure 8,
so this might be expected to lead to clear differentiation
between Mozart and Schubert, if not also between Mozart and other composers).
The resulting prototypes are shown in Figure 3.
Fig. 3. Prototypes for the fingerprint M1.
Sonata in C K545-1 (simple quadruple),
String Quartet in Bb K458-2 (simple triple),
Sonata in D K311-3 (compound duple):
The bar lying between the vertical dashed lines,
here the second bar in each excerpt, is the one considered prototypical.
The main feature of interest is the positioning of the two short notes
just before the middle of the 'real' bar,
or before the 3rd beat in 'real' triple metre
(which is again as near the middle of the bar as the simple divisions
of the metre allow).
This proposed fingerprint will therefore be called for convenience
the 'middle twiddle'.
That admittedly almost childish appellation was chosen because such novelty
provides an advantage when one undertakes a rather large task of score
processing—that is, it will "stand out" well as a name among
the names of various other fingerprints which may arise in future work
involving a number of composers and their fingerprints.
The matching of the score to the curve is again shown by means
of circled letters in the Figure so that, for instance,
the two short notes seem to lead in to the (N)otch phase of the curve.
The Figure illustrates, as did
Figure 2, the relationship between the three
'real' metres, this time with Mozart's own music.
Although the vertical alignment in this and other Figures shows the
progress through the printed score correctly,
the relationships to the curve differ between staves,
as may also the tempo of whole bars and of the music within each bar;
thus the horizontal axis does not in general reflect uniform chronometric time.
The relationship of the score to chronometric time is relevant
in performance studies, not treated here.
Definition of M1
Par 27
The definition of M1 will be written in terms of the 'nominal' metres
4/4, 6/8 and 3/4, assuming B/C = 1.
The corresponding specifications for other metres follow readily.
Definition of M1—the four phases:
d1. The downbeat melody note, or (D) note,
is placed immediately after the bar-line.
d2. The (D) note is not linked by a slur to the previous note.
d3. The duration of the (D) note is a proportion 3/8, 1/3, 1/2
of the bar in the respective 'real' metres.
(Thus only the initial melody note is sounded during the (D) phase.)
a1. Two 16th-notes follow, usually by melodic step progression.
n1. The (N) note follows, usually by melodic step
or descending skip.
n2. The (N) note is harmonized as a consonance
(or a 6-4 chord).
u1. No new melodic note is sounded after the (N) note.
Definition of M1—general requirements:
g1. A certain tempo range is required,
about 1 to 2 seconds per 'real' bar.
g2. An accompaniment, if present, will have no characteristics
that would tend to conflict with the way the melody produces the curve.
g3. Dynamics are about central (not violent, not timid).
Remarks on the definition of M1:
d2. A fresh attack is made on the (D) note,
corresponding to the fact that the top of the curve is pointed
rather than rounded.
A slur into this note might therefore be considered unlikely,
and in fact occurs very seldom in the examples to be shown shortly.
d3. The given proportions refer to the literal score durations,
not necessarily exactly to the durations in a performance
with appropriate nuances.
a1.
These two short notes correspond to a location somewhere near the top
of the (A) phase of the curve.
The fact that these notes run directly into the
(N) note corresponds to the roundedness, rather than pointedness,
of the curve at that stage.
Slurring is found much more often here and elsewhere within the 'real' bar
than between 'real' bars (this is by contrast with the remark on d2 above).
n1. The descending skip to the (N) note may be considered
to define a characteristic subtype,
as will be seen later in the discussion of Figure 4a(i).
Close variants of M1:
d1. The downbeat note may be preceded by a short appoggiatura.
This variant is generally not very distant,
but it has been excluded in some cases—a more consistent approach
to these cases would be desirable in further work.
a1. The movement of the two 16th-notes may occasionally be
disjunct.
u1. One or more new notes may be sounded after the (N) note,
in which case they either follow smoothly from it or introduce a new musical
phrase or gesture.
More distant variants of M1:
a1. Two 32nd-notes, rather than two 8th-notes,
may proceed to beat 3, as in the String Quartet K575-2 bar 34—that is
exceptional, and does not belong to the present fingerprint.
Examples documenting M1
Par 28
A collection of examples of the fingerprint M1 is given as score excerpts
in Figures 4a-e.
The examples are taken from the chosen genres, which are distinguishable
by the appearance of a brace for piano music, a bracket for string quartets,
and vocal text for lieder (except for the accompanimental solo excerpt from
K468 with M1 and later, with M2, K474, K531 and K597).
The collection is organized hierarchically
according to the three categories mentioned earlier:
(i) 'real' metre,
(ii) bars per curve (B/C),
(iii) metrical unit, and finally
(iv) the Köchel catalog order (which is approximately chronological).
Each example covers two curves;
attention is focussed on the music between the dashed vertical lines,
for it is there that M1 is found.
The collection is therefore further organized according to whether
it is the first or the second curve that contains M1,
thereby allowing the vertical alignment of the fingerprints.
A movement (more precisely a tempo/metre segment) may contain several examples
of M1, in which case only one example is shown
here.
14
Par 29
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = 1
We begin with 31 examples in this category.
Since B/C = 1, 'real' and 'nominal' metres coincide here.
The first 13 examples have metre 4/4 (Figure 4a(i)).
In most of these the 16th-notes move by step,
but a quite characteristic drop of a 6th is seen in K080-2, K157-1 and KAnh074,
the latter two of which have identical melodies and harmonies.
Appoggiaturas, apparently harmless to M1, are seen in K333-1
(close variant d1).
The quartet K387-1 also shows signs of the fingerprint M2 (see later).
Fig. 4a(i). Examples of M1. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 4/4:
The next 12 examples have metre 2/4 (Figure 4a(ii)).
In this metre the two short notes are 32nds.
In 7 cases motion to the (N) note is conjunct, in the other 5 disjunct.
K597 has an initial grace-note, indicating a singing nuance (close variant d1).
Fig. 4a(ii). Examples of M1. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 2/4:
The next 6 examples have metre 2/2 (Fig. 4a(iii)).
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = ½
Figure 4b shows 7 examples in this category;
the first four have metre 4/4, the other three 2/2.
In some of these the printed bar is shifted by half a bar,
by comparison with some of the others.
The difference of nominal metre between 4/4 and 2/2 seems to have no effect
in respect of K570-2 (in 4/4) and K468 & K570-3 (in 2/2),
for the music is very similar in these cases.
In K332-2 the 1st bar is the cleaner example, having no appoggiatura,
but the 2nd bar was more convenient for alignment;
this pair of bars confirms the close variant d1.
Examples of M1.
Left: Fig. 4a(iii). Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 2/2.
Right: Fig. 4b. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=½:
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = 2
Only one example in this category has been found in the chosen repertoire, shown in
Figure 4c. The presence of a trill on the downbeat note means that the relevance of this
example here is somewhat uncertain.
Fig. 4c. Examples of M1. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=2:
'Real' simple triple, B/C = 1
Figure 4d shows 8 examples in this category. In four of them further notes follow the
(N) note (close variant u1).
Fig. 4d. Examples of M1. Simple triple, B/C=1:
'Real' simple triple, B/C = ½
No examples were found in this category
(and for this 'real' metre B/C = 2 cannot exist).
'Real' compound duple, B/C = 1
Figure 4e shows 4 examples in this category
(fingerprints for this 'real' metre were not found with other values of B/C).
The movement of the short notes is disjunct in KAnh072 (close variant a1).
Strong similarity is noticed between K311-3, K576-1 and KAnh073.
Fig. 4e. Examples of M1. Compound duple, B/C=1:
An instructive case which was excluded is K331-1 bars 3-4 (see Figure 5).
Here the value of B/C is apparently not 1 but ½.
Features arguing against B/C = 1 are the smoothly prolonged continuation
from the 6-4 chord at the middle of nominal bar 4 and the dynamic markings
(the
sf seems to suggest the crisp upbeat phase
and the
p a new gesture);
confirmation will later be seen in the clear instance of M2 with B/C= ½
found in bar 34 of the same movement
(shown in Figure 5 and later in
Figure 7e).
Most cases did not require such detailed investigation.
Fig. 5. Mozart Piano Sonata K331-1 bars 3-4, 34:
Incidence of the examples
Par 30
The incidence of the examples documenting the fingerprint M1
is shown in Table 3 by date of composition and genre
(together with the incidence for M2, which will be referred to later).
The counting in the Table includes all tempo/metre segments,
even in cases where an initial tempo/metre combination resumes
after a possibly brief departure;
thus the Table underestimates the true incidence a little.
The overall incidence of M1 as tabulated is 23%.
From the percentages in the Table's margins it is seen that the incidence
is well-distributed over time through Mozart's output
and also between the three genres treated here
(excepting only that the incidence in Lieder is lower,
that category having however the fewest total segments
and therefore providing the least reliable estimates).
A lack of uniformity is noticed between the cells within the body of the Table,
and in particular the high incidence in the early String Quartets
compared with the zero incidence in the early Piano Sonatas;
rather than immediately attempt to draw conclusions from that observation,
it will be preferable to await corresponding data
from other genres of Mozart's œuvre.
Table 3.
Incidence of Examples Documenting the Fingerprints M1 and M2
in the Selected Genres of Mozart's Works.
(M1 U M2 means the union of the set of examples of M1 and M2.)
The percentages relate the number of examples
to the number of segments.
|
|
Piano Sonatas |
String Quartets |
Lieder |
Total |
Early
(1761-1778
K001-K300) |
M1
M2
M1 U M2
[Segments] |
0 ( 0.0%)
5 (22.7%)
5 (22.7%)
[22] |
19 (36.5%)
13 (25.0%)
24 (46.2%)
[52] |
0 ( 0.0%)
0 ( 0.0%)
0 ( 0.0%)
[5] |
19 (24.0%)
18 (22.8%)
29 (36.7%)
[79] |
Middle
(1778-1786
K301-K500) |
M1
M2
M1 U M2
[Segments]
|
11 (32.4%)
5 (14.7%)
13 (38.2%)
[34] |
5 (12.5%)
9 (22.5%)
10 (25.0%)
[40] |
2 ( 9.5%)
1 ( 4.8%)
3 (14.3%)
[21] |
18 (18.9%)
15 (15.8%)
26 (27.4%)
[95] |
Late
(1786-1791
K501-K626) |
M1
M2
M1 U M2
[Segments] |
7 (41.2%)
0 ( 0.0%)
7 (41.2%)
[17] |
4 (23.5%)
2 (11.8%)
6 (35.3%)
[17] |
3 (21.4%)
5 (35.7%)
6 (42.9%)
[14] |
14 (29.1%)
7 (14.6%)
19 (39.6%)
[48] |
Total |
M1
M2
M1 U M2
[Segments] |
18 (24.7%)
10 (13.7%)
25 (34.2%)
[73] |
28 (25.7%)
24 (22.0%)
40 (36.7%)
[109] |
5 (12.5%)
6 (15.0%)
9 (22.5%)
[40] |
51 (23.0%)
40 (18.0%)
74 (33.3%)
[222] |
Note: Appendix works (KAnh) have been assigned to their probable dates.
6. The Mozart Fingerprint M2 ('8-5-1 cadence')
Prototypes for M2
Par 31
For the next fingerprint, attention is shifted to another property
of Mozart's conducting shape: its incisiveness and clarity.
As with M1, this property is not shared by Schubert's conducting shape,
so again a ready distinction between the two composers
can reasonably be hoped for.
When the fingerprint M2 occurs it is usually in the bass part of cadences;
prototypes are shown in Figure 6.
Although the pattern contains relatively little detail
and might not so well reflect other properties of Mozart's curve,
it can nevertheless make an important contribution
in helping to determine the value of B/C.
Fig. 6. Prototypes for the fingerprint M2.
Sonata in D K311-1 (simple quadruple),
Sonata in F K332-1 (simple triple),
String Quartet in d K421 (compound duple):
Definition of M2
Par 32
The definition is presented according to the same scheme
as was that of M1.
Definition of M2—the four phases:
d1. The downbeat or (D) note or chord is struck freshly
(i.e. it is not tied to what preceded).
d2. The bass note at (D) has the pitch of the (local) tonic.
d3. No new note appears in the bass voice during the course of this phase.
d4. The (D) note lasts only until the (A) note starts
(the (D) note is not tied forward).
a1. The (A) bass note is struck at a proportion 1/4, 1/3, 1/3 of the bar
in the respective metres.
a2. It has the pitch of the lower dominant.
a3. No new note appears in the bass voice during the course of this phase.
a4. The (A) note lasts only until the (N) note starts
(the (A) note is not tied forward).
n1. The (N) note is struck at a proportion 1/2, 2/3, 1/2 of the bar.
n2. It has the pitch of the lower tonic.
n3. No new note appears in the bass voice during the course of this phase.
u1. A rest appears at the (U) position, except in 'real' triple metre.
u2. It appears at a proportion 3/4, — , 2/3 of the bar.
Definition of M2—general requirements:
These are the same as for M1.
Remarks on the definition of M2:
One of the upper voices very often has an appoggiatura at (D)
with resolution at (N), as in all the examples in Figure 6.
This is an important subtype of M2, but it is not considered separately here.
'Real' compound duple time does not seem to suit M2 nearly so well
as do the other 'real' metres; this is presumably explained by that metre
lending itself relatively little to the required crispness.
u1. The absence of a final rest in 'real' triple metre seems to correspond
to the fusion of the conducting phases (N) and (U);
it is as if the rest were fused with the sounded note,
whereas in the other metres the rest occurs and is felt separately
(see
Par 15).
The other voices do not take part in the 8-5-1 pitch pattern,
for that would considerably change the texture and character.
Close variant of M2:
Other pitch patterns than 8-5-1 may be used, still belonging, however,
to the local tonic triad and having the same rhythm.
To be distinguished from M2:
At broad cadences the pattern's durations may be doubled,
so that it covers not one but two curves
(e.g. K279-3 bars 157-158 compared with bar 10,
as well as K311-3 bars 268-269 compared with bar 48);
such cases are not to be confused with M2,
for the matching of the music to the curve is then very different.
Examples documenting M2
Par 33
Examples for M2 are presented in Figures 7a-f
in the same manner as earlier for M1 (see
Par 28).
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = 1
Figure 7a(i) shows 9 examples in nominal 4/4 metre.
In K155-1 the specified pattern appears in the viola
rather than the cello part.
Variant pitch patterns appear in K080-2, K160-1, K387-1 and K428-1.
Two examples happen to show also M1: K157-1 and K387-1.
Figure 7a(ii) shows 11 examples in nominal 2/4 metre.
Variant pitch patterns appear in K529 and K597.
Examples of M2.
Left: Fig. 7a(i). Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 4/4.
Right: Fig. 7a(ii). Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 2/4:
Figure 7a(iii) shows 4 examples in nominal 2/2 metre.
The pitch pattern is ascending in the top voice in K281-3,
in which the grace notes seem not to interfere with the fingerprint.
Fig. 7a(iii). Examples of M2. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1, 2/2:
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = ½
Figure 7b shows the only example found in this category,
the very clear case K283-2.
Fig. 7b. Example of M2. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=½:
'Real' simple duple/quadruple, B/C = 2
Figure 7c shows two examples in this category,
both having variant pitch patterns.
Fig. 7c. Examples of M2. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=2:
'Real' simple triple, B/C = 1
Figure 7d shows 9 examples in this category.
Variant pitch patterns appear in K170-2 and K589-3.
An example from the Minuet of K173-3 is shown but, to avoid double-counting,
one from the Trio of the same movement was excluded.
Fig. 7d. Examples of M2. Simple triple, B/C=1:
'Real' simple triple, B/C = ½
Figure 7e shows the only example found: K331-1.
This confirms that the temptation to assign B/C = 1 on the basis of bars 3-4
representing M1 should be avoided—see the earlier discussion of
Figure 5.
Fig. 7e. Example of M2. Simple triple, B/C=½:
('Real' simple triple with B/C=2 cannot exist.)
'Real' compound duple, B/C = 1
Figure 7f shows 2 examples (the lowest 2).
In K421-4 the appoggiaturas in the second violin and viola
help to confirm the identification.
The pitch pattern and even the number of notes is varied in K428-2,
but the effect seems clearly that of M2.
'Real' compound duple, B/C = 2
Figure 7f shows, in the upper system,
the only case found in this category: K283-3.
This categorization is supported by the pianist Wanda Landowska's insertion
into the score of one nominal bar (bar 131a, prolonging the a#-b alternation)
where it was required to maintain the 'real' metre with B/C = 2
(see
Nettheim, 1999, pp. 27-28).
Fig. 7f. Examples of M2. Compound duple, B/C=1, 2:
Incidence of the examples
Par 34
Table 3, given earlier,
shows the incidence of M2 as well as of M1.
The incidence of the union of these is also shown
(thus avoiding double-counting of the 17 segments
that contain separate examples of each fingerprint).
In four cases both M1 and M2 are seen in a single example:
K157-1 bars 33-34 (
Fig. 7a(i)),
K387-1a bar 10 (
Fig. 7a(i)),
K589-3 bars 7-8 (a variant of M1,
Fig. 7d),
K421-4 bars 23-24 (again a variant of M1,
Fig 7f);
these too have been counted only once.
The overall incidence of the union of M1 and M2 is 33%,
reasonably high considering the restrictive approach taken
to the definitions in this first study.
The Table's margins show fair uniformity over time and genre,
while the individual cells naturally show some variability.
On the whole, the collection of examples seems fairly convincing to me.
Formal comparison with the incidence of the Schubert fingerprint
proposed in earlier work is not available, because in that work the sampling,
though wider ranging and more extensive than here,
was unavoidably less controlled; an informal estimate was indicated
as 50% overall (
Nettheim, 1998, section 3).
Summary of values of B/C
in the examples of M1 and M2
Par 35
For each 'real' metre the number of segments having at least one
fingerprint example is shown in Table 4 for each value of B/C
(the use of the union of the set of examples of M1
and the set of examples of M2 avoids double counting).
The totals show that 83.7% had B/C = 1, while 12.2% had B/C = ½
and 4.1% B/C = 2.
These percentages are not necessarily indicative of the percentages
in the population of Mozart's segments;
an estimate of that is not available as we have not yet ascertained the value
of B/C for all segments in the population.
They may nevertheless serve as a preliminary estimate.
It was perhaps to be expected that B/C = 1 would predominate,
the less direct notations appearing less frequently.
The percentages found here are quite similar to those found in the examples
of the Schubert fingerprint proposed in earlier work:
74.7%, 16.0% and 9.3% respectively
(
Nettheim, 1998, Section 3, Table 3a).
Table 4. Number of Examples of M1 U M2 by Bars per Curve (B/C)
and 'Real' metre.
(M1 U M2 means the union of the set of examples of M1 and of M2.)
|
Bars per Curve (B/C) |
|
½ |
1 |
2 |
Total |
'Real' metre
| Simple duple/quadruple |
8 |
41 |
2 |
51 |
Simple triple |
1 |
15 |
— |
16 |
Compound duple |
0 |
6 |
1 |
7 |
Total
(Percent) |
9
(12.2%) |
62
(83.7%) |
3
(4.1%) |
74
(100.0%) |
Par 36
For each 'nominal' metre and each value of B/C,
the number of segments having an example of M1 or M2 or both
is shown in Table 5.
Again necessarily restricting attention to the examples so far collected,
it is seen that two metres, 2/2 and 3/4, always show B/C = 1,
so that 'real' and 'nominal' metre are the same.
Whether future research will confirm that Mozart's music in these metres
provides reliable clues to the determination of the value of B/C remains,
however, to be seen.
Table 5. Number of Examples of M1 U M2 by Bars per Curve (B/C)
and 'Nominal' metre.
(M1 U M2 means the union of the set of examples of M1 and of M2.)
|
Bars per Curve (B/C) |
|
½ |
1 |
2 |
Total |
'Nominal' metre |
2/2 |
|
9 |
|
9 |
2/4 |
|
18 |
2 |
20 |
3/4 |
— |
15 |
|
15 |
4/4 |
5 |
14 |
3 |
22 |
3/8 |
— |
0 |
0 |
0 |
6/8 |
1 |
6 |
1 |
8 |
Total |
6 |
62 |
6 |
74 |
7. Tempo
Par 37
It is of interest to examine the relationship between Mozart's tempo
indications and the values of B/C occurring in his scores,
to see whether the indications correspond to 'real' or to 'nominal' bars
(or to neither of those).
Data covering Mozart's whole output is available in
Marty (1988) for the
tempo indications but is unfortunately not presently available for the
values of B/C (Marty did not systematically treat these latter,
instead attempting to find systematic behaviour in the relation between
the indications and the note values used in the compositions).
We therefore investigate this relationship
just in the present examples of M1 and M2:
the data, read off from the examples in Figures 4 and 7, is summarized in
Table 6.
15
Table 6. Tempo Indications Generally Appearing in the Examples of M1 and M2
for each 'Real' metre and each Number of Bars per Curve (B/C).
|
'Real' metre |
|
Simple duple/quadruple |
Simple triple |
Compound duple |
B/C |
½ |
Andante |
Andante |
(No observations) |
1 |
Allegro |
Full range* |
Allegro |
2 |
Presto |
— |
Presto |
* Mostly Menuetto in the examples.
Par 38
We now briefly discuss the entries in the Table's cells.
When B/C = ½ it is seen from
Table 4
that 9 segments are involved;
of these, the only case that is faster than Andante is K570-3 at Allegretto.
When B/C = 2, 3 segments are involved, admittedly a small number,
whose tempos are Presto, Presto and Allegro assai.
These two extremes, the first and last rows of Table 6,
are the most revealing for the present purpose.
It remains to consider the (less critical) middle row, where B/C=1.
In simple duple/quadruple metre 41 segments are involved; the great majority
of these have a variety of Allegro or a slightly slower tempo, the only
apparently differing ones being K597 Etwas langsam and K589-2 Larghetto.
In simple triple metre 15 segments are involved,
showing only one case of a non-central tempo: K169-1 at Molto allegro.
In compound duple metre 6 segments are involved, showing 3 Allegros,
an Andante con moto and two with no tempo indication—there
are no extremes here.
Par 39
For a more thorough investigation, the authenticity
of all the published tempo indications would need to be checked,
as well as any other relevant circumstances applying in particular cases.
But the conclusion seems inescapable,
though drawn just from the present limited data,
that Mozart's tempo indications correspond in general not to 'real'
but to 'nominal' bars.
Thus Presto means that it is the notated bars that proceed rapidly,
the 'real' bars proceeding only half as rapidly.
Similarly for Andante and slower tempos it is the notated bars
that proceed at the indicated speed,
the 'real' bars proceeding only half as slowly.
It is to be noted that the same general conclusion
had been reached earlier for Schubert's verbal tempo indications
(
Nettheim, 1998, Section 4).
These conclusions have practical importance,
for they imply that the conducting tempos for both composers vary less
than if the indications had corresponded to 'real' bars.
More specifically, for these composers
Presto music is generally not as fast, when B/C=2, as might have been assumed
if the B/C factor had not been taken into account,
and similarly Larghetto is generally not as slow, when B/C=½,
as might have been assumed.
Par 40
It might be tempting to draw a further conclusion from the Table
by reversing the direction of implication:
that in order to determine the value of B/C
one need only look at the tempo indication,
except when 'real' simple triple metre is a possibility.
To draw that conclusion would, however, be risky until further data
is available to confirm the relationships suggested by the Table and,
even then, it is unlikely to apply without exceptions arising;
a separate investigation of the prevailing musical features therefore remains
the preferred method for determining the value of B/C for a given segment.
8. Comparison between Mozart and Schubert
Par 41
Now that the works of two composers have been studied
from the present point of view (with a fair though not complete coverage),
the two sets of results may be compared.
Cross-matches: [A]
Incidence of M1 and M2 in Schubert
Par 42
A cross-match is regarded here as a passage found in the scores of one
composer that could convincingly be executed according to the conducting curve
of another composer.
It is to be expected that many excerpts containing more or less neutral
material could have been written by either composer;
this material may in an extreme case consist simply
of a whole ('real') bar's rest,
or in less extreme cases a simple chord or scale or repeated-note passage.
Such neutral excerpts are not of interest here, and we will pass them by.
Even a non-neutral cross-match does not by itself constitute a refutation
of the approach taken in this paper:
instead, it challenges the researcher to look closely enough
into the excerpts to find features
distinguishing the two musical personalities involved.
If, of course, one were unable to find such features,
the approach would to that extent have failed.
Par 43
The search for cross-matches requires, in the present case,
the attempt to work through Schubert's scores after having mentally
(and perhaps physically) installed Mozart's conducting curve,
so as to see whether any excerpts from those scores are to a fair extent
compatible with Mozart's curve.
That is a somewhat unnatural task for a musician,
but for this purpose it must be carried out diligently.
The subjectivity of the method will be minimized, as before,
by carefully examining the excerpts thus brought to light
in relation to the already defined fingerprints and curve characteristics.
Par 44
The definitions of M1 and M2 will need to be modified to some extent
for the purpose of finding instances of them in a work by Schubert.
For instance, the textures of the writing will usually differ between composers,
in some cases even the available instruments or their range,
and that alone may make strict cross-matches unlikely.
Further, the value of B/C will need to be known,
not as it may be determined according to the writing of Schubert,
even though Schubert was in fact the composer, but, if it were possible,
according to the writing of Mozart, the 'null hypothesis' composer.
Par 45
The field of the comparison must be determined too:
the null hypothesis may be that all of Schubert's output could have been
written by Mozart,
16
that a given work could have been, a given movement, segment,
portion of a segment, excerpt covering just one conducting curve
(of what would be Mozart's shape), or an even smaller unit.
If the music of a single curve is at issue it follows that,
as one cannot determine B/C from just one curve,
one must determine it by conducting also the surrounding material
as if it were by Mozart,
even though that single curve might be the only one whose music appears to fit
the Mozart curve.
Par 46
The problems just discussed seem to rule out the possibility of a strict
definition of a cross-match.
I therefore decided to search in Schubert's works for instances which might
informally be described as "something like" the Mozart fingerprints.
It is of interest to obtain a (necessarily informal) idea of the frequency
of such instances;
in order to err on the side of overestimating that frequency,
I noted cases of even slight similarity,
and accordingly use the term 'cross-match candidate'.
Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert
Par 47
Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert are shown in Figures 8a-e.
The survey is confined to the solo piano sonatas.
Par 48
'Real' simple duple/quadruple
Three candidates were found with B/C = 1 (Figure 8a).
In D566-1 bars 3-6, 63-66, the melody note held throughout the 3rd and 4th beats is not
typical of Mozart's M1 figure and fits it less well. In D845-1 bar 162, 165 many features
contradict M1.
Finally, in D459-1 bars 1, 3, the opening RH figure
is the same as in K576-3 bars 1, 3
(see Figure 9, which is placed within the series 8a-e).
Schubert's crescendo/diminuendo swelling across the 2nd and 4th bar-lines
is one feature differentiating the gesture from Mozart's
as it suggests a less crisp downbeat.
That opening figure is apparently not very important to Schubert,
for it never recurs in the movement
(the recapitulation begins at bar 80 with plain RH octaves).
Fig. 8a. Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1:
Fig. 9. Mozart K576-3 bars 1-4, Schubert D459-1 bars 1-4:
One candidate was found with B/C = 2 (Figure 8b).
In D625-4 bars 35-36, 210-211 the duplication of the (N) figure in the LH
produces a less nimble effect than in Mozart.
Fig. 8b. Cross-match candidate of M1 in Schubert. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=2:
'Real' simple triple
Four distinct candidates were found, all with B/C=1 (Figure 8c).
In D557-1 bars 2, 6 the continuation does not fit well with item u1
of the definition of M1.
In D567-1 bars 45, 53, 171, 179 = D568-1 bars 45, 53, 190, 198
(in which we refer to the 'real' bar-line with B/C = 1,
understanding this music according to Mozart,
whereas possibly B/C = 2 according to Schubert)
the crescendo/diminuendo across the bar-line seems un-Mozartean,
as it did in
Figure 9.
17
The remaining candidates are D894-2 bar 141 (in octaves), 143, 144
and D960-2 bars 36, 40, 129, 133;
the context provided by the surrounding material seems to differentiate
these from Mozartean writing, yet by themselves they match M1 reasonably well.
Fig. 8c. Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert. Simple triple, B/C=1:
'Real' compound duple
Two candidates were found with B/C = 1 (Figure 8d). D568-4 bars 95, 97 and D604
bars 2, 35 introduce no features beyond those seen in earlier candidates.
Fig. 8d. Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert. Compound duple, B/C=1:
Three candidates were found with B/C = 2 (Figure 8e).
In D537-3 bars 210-213, D575-4 bars 81-86, 307-316, and D845-3 bars 1-2
a short repeated figure does not lead to a good match with M1;
the latter two candidates contradict item d2 of the definition of M1.
Fig. 8e. Cross-match candidates of M1 in Schubert. Compound duple, B/C=2:
Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert
Par 49
Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert are shown in Figures 10a-d,
where the survey is again restricted to the solo piano sonatas.
Par 50
'Real' simple duple/quadruple
One candidate was found with B/C = 1 (Figure 10a).
In D664-1 bar 133 the first bass note is held through,
contradicting item d4 of the definition of M2
and thereby losing Mozart's incisiveness.
Fig. 10a. Cross-match candidate of M2 in Schubert. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1:
Two candidates were found with B/C = 2 (Figure 10b).
In D157-1 bars 103-104, 250-251
(also in the earlier version D154-1 bars 85-86),
the solid chords in the RH make the music more heavy-handed than for Mozart.
In D958-2 bars 114-115, the pitch pattern is varied
and the last chord is twice as long as in M2.
Fig. 10b. Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=2:
'Real' simple triple
Two candidates were found, both with B/C = 1 (Figure 10c).
In D850-3 bar 51 and D959-3 bar 22 the pitches are inverted.
In the first case Schubert appears to be experimenting with B/C = 2
in the RH against B/C = 1 in the LH,
creating a special accentuation foreign to Mozart.
In the second case the first bass note is held through the bar,
contradicting definition item d4.
Fig. 10c. Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert. Simple triple, B/C=1:
'Real' compound duple
Three candidates were found. One has B/C = 1 (Figure 10d).
D557-3 bars 19-20 is a fairly close match to K428-2 bars 34-35
(shown in
Figure 7f)
but both have 4 chords rather than 3
and so are not fully convincing representations of M2.
Fig. 10d. Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert. Compound duple, B/C=1:
Two have B/C = 2 (Figure 10e):
D157-3 bars 19-20, 67-68 and D625-3 bars 47-48,
the latter with a different pitch pattern.
If it is accepted that compound duple time is Mozart's least congenial one,
then these three examples would not have been expected
to be very convincing candidates.
Fig. 10e. Cross-match candidates of M2 in Schubert. Compound duple, B/C=2:
Cross-matches: [B]
Incidence of S1 in Mozart
Par 51
We must now attempt to grasp Schubert's musical personality.
The Becking curve for Schubert is shown in Figure 11,
whose accompanying text is "führen und schwingen"
(guide and swing).
18
The Becking curve for Mozart was shown in
Figure 1.
These curves differ greatly in orientation (vertical vs horizontal),
in contour, and in the associated feeling qualities;
a reader who executes them with conviction will more fully grasp
the significance of the differences between them.
Fig. 11. Becking curve for Schubert, with regions
(reproduced from Nettheim, 1998, Fig. 3):
The usage of 'nominal' and 'real' metres by the two composers differs too;
we do not reproduce here the details given in
Nettheim (1998, section 2)
but mention in particular that it seems that,
whereas 'real' duple/quadruple suited Mozart specially well,
'real' compound duple suited Schubert better.
A Schubert fingerprint, S1, was proposed and defined in detail in
Nettheim (1998, section 3)
as a 'prepared appoggiatura' figure.
The somewhat extensive details of its definition will not be reproduced here,
but prototypes for each main 'real' metre are shown in Figure 12
(in each case the second 'real' bar contains S1).
Fig. 12. Prototypes for the fingerprint S1.
Moment Musical D780/6 (compound duple),
Das Wirtshaus D911/21 (simple duple),
String Quartet in C D046-2 (simple triple)
(reproduced from Nettheim, 1998, Figure 5):
Cross-match candidates of S1 in Mozart
Par 52
Cross-match candidates of S1 in Mozart are shown in Figures 13a-d.
The survey is again confined to the solo piano sonatas.
Par 53
'Real' simple duple/quadruple
One candidate was found with B/C = 1 (Figure 13a).
In K330-3 bar 169 the texture is thinner than is typical for Schubert,
a reservation which will naturally be likely to apply
to all or most of these candidates.
More significantly it is seen that,
whereas Mozart has repeated the note f' on the downbeat of bar 169,
Schubert would, according to our knowledge of S1,
more likely have moved to e' there,
resulting in a smoother progression at the middle of the bar
and thereby corresponding better to Schubert's than to Mozart's
conducting shape.
Such an experiment of re-composing an excerpt from the music of one composer
to introduce a feature of the other composer is often instructive.
Fig. 13a. Cross-match candidate of S1 in Mozart. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=1:
One candidate was found with B/C = ½ (Figure 13b).
In K282-1 bar 36 a Mozartean, rather than Schubertian, feature
seems to be the slur over the notes g-ab in the first half of the bar,
which does not allow the ab to function as a typical beginning
of a Schubertian gesture.
Fig. 13b. Cross-match candidate of S1 in Mozart. Simple duple/quadruple, B/C=½:
'Real' simple triple
One candidate was found with B/C = 1:
K475-1 bars 153-154 (also bar 157) (Figure 13c).
The sfp accent in the middle of bar 153
perhaps militates against a Schubertian rendition.
Fig. 13c. Cross-match candidate of S1 in Mozart. Simple triple, B/C=1:
'Real' compound duple
Three candidates were found, all with B/C = 1: K280-2 bars 8, 42, 44, 46
(bar 44 is shown); K332-3 bar 236; and K576-1 bar 129 (Figure 13d).
In the first of these, Mozart's rests are a distinguishing feature,
the first one acting in opposition to a smooth Schubertian downbeat—this
is highlighted in the comparison of similar musical material by the two
composers shown in Figure 14.
The fact that these three candidates match Schubert somewhat better
than those in other 'real' metres might be due in part to
'real' compound duple metre being less sympathetic to Mozart's shape
and more sympathetic to Schubert's.
Similarly, a close match in Schubert to Mozart had been seen in
Figure 9 in simple duple/quadruple metre,
a metre less sympathetic to Schubert than to Mozart.
Fig. 13d. Cross-match candidates of S1 in Mozart. Compound duple, B/C=1:
Fig. 14. Mozart K280-2 bars 43-44, Schubert D780-6 bars 40-43:
Par 54
As so few candidates for S1 occur in Mozart's scores,
it may be instructive to describe a few cases of appoggiatura figures in Mozart
which are not considered to be candidates,
together with the reasons for their non-candidature.
These reasons are not based on mere pedantry,
but ultimately on the extent to which the scores match
the given conducting curve, which I have attempted to make explicit
with the aid of fingerprint definitions.
Some features of many of Mozart's appoggiaturas,
compared with many of Schubert's, are shown in Table 7.
Table 7. Some Features of Many Appoggiaturas in Schubert and Mozart
(Corresponding items from the definition of Schubert's fingerprint S1
in Nettheim, 1998 are shown in parentheses)
|
Feature |
Schubert |
Mozart |
A |
Approach pitch |
(d2) Same as main note |
Different from main note |
B |
Approach length |
(u1) As specified in S1 |
Often shorter, occasionally longer |
C |
Appoggiatura length |
(f2) As specified in S1: long |
Longer or shorter |
D |
Activity during appoggiatura |
(f1) None |
Some |
E |
Resolution direction |
(r1) Usually downward |
Quite often upward |
F |
Bass movement on resolution |
(r2) None |
Some |
Here are a few examples having the tabulated features
(a more complete list is available from the author):
(A) K279-1 bar 5, (B) K281-1 bar 40, (C) K457-3 bars 175-176,
(D) K283-2 bar 4b, (E) K311-1 bar 39, (F) K280-2 bar 24.
Many examples have more than one of these features, e.g. K283-2 bar 39b (B,D).
In particular, short appoggiaturas are particularly characteristic of Mozart
rather than of Schubert.
A thorough and systematic examination of Mozart's appoggiaturas lies,
however, beyond the scope of the present paper.
Par 55
To conclude the comparison between Mozart and Schubert,
we show in Figure 15 an excerpt for each composer,
intended to represent each one's curve well
(for the Mozart excerpt it is assumed that B/C=1,
for the Schubert excerpt 2).
These excerpts are thus chosen to be as
distinct as possible
in the present sense.
(On the other hand, the excerpts in
Figures
9 and
14
were chosen to be as
similar as possible,
so those pairs are of the kind so well selected and instructively
differentiated by
Becking, ibid).
Fig. 15. Mozart K576-3 bars 1-4, Schubert D780/6 bars 1-4:
Each excerpt contains a fingerprint,
and each composer's "favourite" 'real' metre has been chosen.
These excerpts seem to me to sum up the two composers' musical personalities
about as well as can be managed in such short excerpts,
and to contrast them well.
(Other features than the presently-studied fingerprints might play a role here,
especially Mozart's short appoggiaturas in bars 2 and 4 mentioned earlier
in connection with
Table 7.)
An attempt to execute either excerpt according the other's curve,
whether in performance or in thought, will, I believe,
be found to violate the musical character of each composer.
This could be assumed to be readily grasped by a person
sensitive to this music,
but it can now be made more explicit
by referring to the matching of features of the excerpts,
and especially their fingerprints, to corresponding features of the
curves.
19
9. Comparison between Mozart and Haydn
(and between Schubert and Haydn)
Par 56
The main purpose of this section is to look briefly at the comparison
between Mozart and Haydn in respect of the fingerprints M1 and M2.
The music of these two composers is usually thought to be somewhat similar
in general terms,
so the extent to which the present method can distinguish between them
is of some interest.
However, as this comparison is not central to the purpose of the present paper,
we do not attempt a thorough study of Haydn's music at this time,
we do not propose a fingerprint for him,
and we do not look for Haydn-like figures in Mozart's scores.
Instead, we will look for cross-matches of M1 and M2 in Haydn's scores.
We will also take the opportunity to notice any evidence in Haydn's scores
(however unlikely it may seem
a priori) of Schubert's fingerprint S1;
that constitutes a secondary purpose of this section.
The greater the number of composers involved,
the more difficult is the mental task of searching through the scores
for all the possible cross-matches
(with a certain value of B/C to be determined in each case),
so the present task is not entirely easy to carry out.
Attention is once again restricted to the solo piano sonatas
(
Haydn, 1962).
Par 57
To save space, detailed results will not be given here.
The general results are that cross-matches in Haydn to the fingerprints
M1 or S1 occur only rarely;
cross-matches in Haydn to M2 occur somewhat more commonly than that,
but still M2 is a good deal less common in Haydn than in Mozart.
One example of each of these cross-match candidates is given in Figure 16
(note that H44 has B/C = ½).
Here we have shown quite close candidates.
However, the more distant candidates, which are more commonly found,
may be more instructive (there is not space for them here);
for instance, candidates for M1 often show melodic notes
following the (N) note,
candidates for M2 often have the last bass note twice as long as in M2,
and candidates for S1 often have double rather than single appoggiaturas.
Fig. 16. Cross-match candidates in Haydn of M1 & M2 (left) and S1 (right):
It may finally be remarked that Haydn's prepared appoggiaturas often differ
from Schubert's through having a much shorter preparation note
or through the bass moving at the resolution of the appoggiatura.
10. Conclusions and future research
Par 58
The basis of the present work has been Becking's notion
of the composer-specific conducting curve.
To what Becking provided I have attempted to add several features:
(a) the fingerprint as an aid to systematic score study
and to the matching of scores to curves,
(b) the systematic treatment of metre, and
(c) the investigation of relations between the number of notated bars
per conducting curve (B/C) and tempo.
The attempt to encapsulate Mozart's musical personality
in terms of fingerprints found in his scores
seems to have had a successful beginning here.
The contrast with Schubert's musical personality
also seems clearly drawn in similar terms.
The contrast with Haydn's musical personality
has been attempted so far only in a cursory manner.
Mozart's tempo indications, like Schubert's, seem to relate generally
to notated bars so, when B/C is taken into account,
Presto music is in many cases not as fast as might otherwise have been assumed,
and conversely the music having slow indications is in many cases not as slow
as might have been assumed.
Par 59
It is anticipated that future work will include the attempt
to add further fingerprints (M3..., S2...)
to the ones already found for Mozart and Schubert,
and the expansion of the coverage of those composers' works in the surveys.
Other methods of determining the number of notated bars per conducting curve
are to be pursued;
such methods will be used alongside the fingerprint method.
The similar treatment of other composers and comparisons among them
should naturally be attempted.
Perhaps further into the future,
the specification of performance nuances
appropriate to the composers studied
will be able to be explored on the solid basis
of the relationship between the scores and the conducting curves.
Appendix.
Some relevant excerpts from the writings of Ingmar Bengtsson (1920-1991)
Par 60
Ingmar Bengtsson's view was similar to that adopted in the present
paper, though he apparently had not followed it up in a similar way;
here we provide two relevant
excerpts.
Par 61
[A]
Bengtsson (1961):
{p.58:} It is this concept of "total rhythm" which writers like G.
Becking, W. Danckert, and, before them, the Sievers-Saran school and others,
were trying to characterize, and whose existence we must accept regardless of
possible objections to these writers' descriptive methods. We are aware that
there exist both "basic attitudes" or "behavioral types" with regard to rhythm
which are characteristic of different historical periods, and also individual
rhythmic "behavior patterns" which differ for Mozart and Beethoven, for
Mendelssohn and Schumann, for Stravinsky and Schoenberg, and so on. The
relevant distinguishing features of these patterns, which are time-bound and
often personality-linked must be assumed to be somehow "hidden in the
notation" because of the fact that one can, with the notation as a point of
departure (complemented by conventions for reading and performing which
cannot be discussed here), deliver interpretations which give the listeners
"spontaneous" impressions of these stylistic patterns and differences.
Par 62
[B]
Bengtsson (1955):
Chapter VIII : The Critical Method and Aims of the Stylistic Analyses
... In the so-called persontypology (Analysis of Personality Types) which was
founded by J. and 0. Rutz and which was adopted for musical purposes by
Gustav Becking and Werner Danckert amongst others, an important attempt is
made to differentiate between and describe a number of basic human types,
which were observed from both the point of view of physical condition and
from the point of view of certain expressive outlets including the artistic. It is
simple to demonstrate the metaphysical weaknesses of this doctrine, but the
existence of the qualitative differences which have been assigned the name
"Personalkonstanten" (constant characteristics of personality) cannot be denied.
And without doubt, Becking in his study of rhythm has laid his finger on one of
the most vital factors to which attention can be drawn if one wishes to arrive at
an assessment of a composer's personal style: as the basic dimension of music is
time, its rhythmic character assumes primary importance. The rhythmic
structure of a piece is of course not solely the product of time-relationships, but
is also affected by melodic and harmonic factors. In the constant characteristics
of musical personality rhythm however plays a dominant role.
The question is then, if these features of individuality are demonstrable
in the musical notation. The notation of a work is not the same thing as the
work itself. However, it serves as a point of departure for all knowledge of that
work. What we can say with immediate and often relatively sure accuracy are
such things as the pitch and time-relationships of the notes. But for the more
refined and subtle characteristics of a work with which we are concerned here
(discussed in chapter IX), there seem to be no means of notation, and the
question arises whether observation of such characteristics is solely the product
of a more or less subjective interpretation of indications in the score. If this is
the case, it is not possible to verify them empirically.
It is the view of the present author however, that all those statements
are capable of empirical verification that can be demonstrated through certain
defined relationships found between various elements in the score. ...
References
Becking, Gustav (1928).
Der Musikalische Rhythmus als Erkenntnisquelle.
(Musical rhythm as a source of insight.) Augsberg: Benno Filser.
Bengtsson, I. (1955).
J.H. Roman och hans instrumentalmusik: käll- och stilkritiska
studier.
(J.H.Roman and his instrumental music: critical studies of source
and style.) Sweden: Uppsala University (Studia musicologica upsaliensia)
(In Swedish.) English-language summary published
on the Internet at
http://www.geocities.com/johanhelmichroman/romans.html
(current at 24th September 2009) under the title
"Johan Helmich Roman (1694-1758)".
Bengtsson, I. (1961).
"On relationships between tonal and rhythmic structures in Western
multipart music." Svensk Tidskrift för Musikforskning
43, 49-76.
Clynes, M. (1983). Expressive microstructure in music,
linked to living qualities.
Royal Swedish Academy of Music Publication No. 39.
Cope, D. (1999). "Signatures and Earmarks:
Computer Recognition of Patterns in Music.
In W. B. Hewlett & E. Selfridge-Field (Eds.),
Melodic Similarity: Concepts, Procedures, and Applications
(pp.129-138). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Edlund, Bengt (2002). "Schubert's Promising Note:
Further Exercises in Musical Hermeneutics".
(Unpublished ms.)
Ehrenzweig, A. (1967).
The Hidden Order of Art. England: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.
(Reprinted St. Albans, Herts, England: Paladin, 1970.)
Gjerdingen, R. O. (1991).
"Defining a prototypical utterance." Psychomusicology 10, 127-139.
Haydn, J. (1962-1972).
Klaviersonaten. München: Henle (G. Feder, Ed., 3 vols.).
Marty, J-P. (1988).
The Tempo Indications of Mozart.
New Haven: Yale University Press.
Mozart, W. A. (1990).
Neue Ausgabe sämtlicher Werke.
Kassel: Bärenreiter (20 vols.).
Nettheim, N. (1999).
"The Deceptive Simplicity of Mozart's Sonata K. 283."
Clavier 38/10, 25-28.
Nettheim, N. (2003).
"A Composer-specific Conducting Simulation." (Submitted)
Randel, D., ed. (1986).
The New Harvard Dictionary of Music.
Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Schubert, F. (1884-97).
Complete Works. (Mandyczewski, Brahms, Hellmesberger, et al.,
Eds.) Leipzig: Breitkopf and Härtel.
(Reprinted New York: Dover, 1965-69.)
Shove, P. & Repp, B. H. (1995).
"Musical motion and performance: theoretical and empirical
perspectives." In J. Rink (Ed.),
The Practice of Performance: Studies in Musical Interpretation
(pp.55-83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Footnotes
1. The concept of multiple fingerprints is consistent with the real-world
notion of fingerprint, for each of our fingers has its own pattern.
Any one musical pattern might not distinguish the composer's music
with perfect clarity, though together they may do so.
A pattern that contributes to joint differentiation without being
fully distinctive itself might better be called an "indicator"
than a "fingerprint",
but we will not need to indulge in that subtlety here.
Compare also the terms "signature" and "earmark" used by Cope (1999).
2. Performance nuances were studied by Clynes (1983), who took up
Becking's idea only in its most general terms:
that mismatching of composers to curves leads to stumbling
in the execution of the curves.
Clynes did not, however, adopt the details of the Becking curves.
In particular, the hierarchical operation of the curve over nested sections
of music, proposed by Clynes, has no basis in Becking's theory.
3. Becking's work, too, was not concerned with the point of view of
academic psychology (1928, p.7).
Both Becking's study and the present one may, however,
have implications in that discipline.
An application has been made
to the selection of an appropriate excerpt for the computer animation
of the conducting beat (Nettheim, 2003).
The Schubert paper Nettheim (1998)
has also found application in Edlund (2002).
4. The physiological mechanism is not known,
and some people might be aware of it more mentally than physically,
while for others it might perhaps be entirely subconscious;
these matters do not need to be dealt with for the present purpose.
Methods designed to assist people to become sensitive to this phenomenon
are being developed (cf. Nettheim, 2003);
this is not so far customarily taught.
5. Another possible method involves the observation of the size of
compositional units recognizable as phrases (typically four 'real' bars),
periods (typically eight 'real' bars), and so on;
my efforts to apply this method in the case of Mozart
have so far proven inconclusive.
6. An interesting case is seen in Bei Männern from
Die Zauberflöte K620/7.
Mozart first wrote the music in 3/8 and subsequently rewrote it
representing each pair of original 3/8 bars in a single bar of 6/8;
a significant difference was apparently intended here,
though possibly not of the 'essential' type,
for the rewritten version begins not with a full bar of 6/8
but just with a half bar.
7. The Becking curve for a given composer should in any case be understood
to represent not a single curve but a family of such curves
having more or less similar shape and character,
for small modifications are always possible according to local circumstances.
8. Our distinction between 'real' and 'nominal' metre
is surely not novel,
yet dictionary definitions of 'metre' do not always acknowledge it.
Thus Randel (1986, p.489) appears to handle
only the case B/C = 1:
"Meter. The pattern in which a steady succession of rhythmic pulses is
organized; ...
One complete pattern or its equivalent in length is termed a measure or bar
and in musical notation is enclosed between two bar lines".
It is Randel's fixed number "two" with which we take issue.
On a more philosophical level, it should perhaps be mentioned that our use
of the term 'real' metre is intended simply to contrast with 'nominal' metre,
not to preempt other possible theories by insisting that ultimate 'reality'
has here been found.
Finally, where we depart farthest from conventional treatments
is in requiring a separate study of the phenomena for each composer.
9. Of Becking's 13 examples from Mozart only one is in 6/8,
and he regarded that one as 2/4 with triplets
(ibid, p.194).
10. Series IX group 25, series VIII group 20 section 1,
and series III group 8, respectively.
The piano sonata sometimes known as K547a is not included.
11. Support for this approach is seen in some writings by Ingmar Bengtsson given in the
Appendix.
12. The need for such understanding was minimized in the largely automated
approach of Cope (1999), who however pursued rather different aims.
13. Gjerdingen also provided a discussion of the problems of defining
prototypes in general.
His Figures 5 and 8 include instances of the fingerprint M1
to be defined later, and his Figure 3 includes instances of M2.
14. For bar numbering I do not increment the count for 2nd-ending bars,
and I do not reset the counting in successive Variations or in Trio sections.
15. The assignment of the values of B/C had been made for all the examples
before this Table was drawn up.
Reviewing now the interesting case of the assignment of K331-1 to B/C
= ½ (Figures 5 and 7e),
we see that this assignment is further supported (though not proven)
by the Table on account of its Andante marking.
16. The question is, then, whether one would have any success
if one took the scores of Schubert and, as if by mistake,
searched there for Mozart fingerprints by the same method as was used above
in searching in Mozart's own scores.
17. In connection with such examples it might be asked whether Mozart
ever used crescendo/diminuendo across the ('real') bar-line.
The answer, for the solo piano sonatas,
is apparently that he did so only in K281-2 bars 2-4, 60-62;
but as he seldom used diminuendo or decrescendo in this repertoire,
the test cannot be carried out with accuracy.
18. The curve for Schubert is shown here in the direction understood
by the present writer, though for Becking it ran in the opposite direction,
that is, towards the body on the main beat; as the shape is unchanged, however,
this issue does not affect the present purpose.
19. Further light of the same kind may be shed by a comparison
of prototypical fingerprint excerpts in each of the three main 'real' metres
(Figures 3, 6
and 12).
Acknowledgements
The author thanks Dr. Bruno H. Repp for detailed comments,
and Magnus Svanfeldt for kindly giving permission to reproduce in the
Appendix an excerpt from his English-language abstract of Bengtsson (1955).
Nettheim home